CHRISTOPH VOLTER
Working memory is central to our mental lives. It allows us to keep track of a series of previous events or actions. For instance, we use it to add up the cost of our shopping or to remember the beginning of this sentence at its end. Some scientists argue that, compared to other animals, it is particularly developed in humans. But how do chimpanzees, one of our closest relatives, compare?
Along with other researchers from the University of St Andrews, the Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology, and the University of Veterinary Medicine Vienna, I set out to answer this question. Previous studies showed that chimpanzees have excellent short-term and long-term memory abilities. But, so far, little is known about their working memory abilities. Working memory allows us not only to keep something in mind for a few seconds but also to manipulate information and update the information we have.
It’s attention demanding process and so if we get distracted, we often forget what we kept in mind just a moment ago. The only information held in working memory can be stored in long-term memory, where it can be held indefinitely. These different stages of human memory act as a filter, helping us to store information without being flooded. Working memory has been repeatedly invoked as one of the key aspects that separate humans from their closest living relatives. But our study provides evidence that chimpanzees possess similar working memory abilities to humans. They’re able to perform at a level comparable to seven-year-old children in a working memory task that requires them to constantly update their memory. These findings are contrary to previous claims which said that chimpanzees’ working memory capacity is fundamentally different from our own.
Standard working memory tasks for people often means they have to continuously update their working memory. So to test the working memory of chimpanzees, we set 13 zoo chimpanzees a task in which they could search for food in a number of small, opaque boxes. The chimpanzees were familiar with this type of task and could choose whether they wanted to participate or not. The chimpanzees first watched how pieces of food were hidden in all of these boxes. They then started to search for the food items by pointing at the boxes one by one. If a chosen box contained food, the chimpanzees received this food reward and the now empty box was then placed back in between the other boxes. After each choice, the boxes were covered with a screen for 15 seconds.
This prevented the chimpanzees from visually tracking the boxes that still contained food. The chimpanzees would have to remember which box they checked. After each choice they would have to update their memory. Only if they managed to continuously keep in mind which boxes they had already chosen, and which boxes still contained food, would they be able to retrieve all of the food items within the number of choices that they got. It’s this memory updating requirement that makes this task different from short-term memory tasks.
Depending on the chimpanzees’ ability to avoid revisiting a box they had already chosen, we increased the difficulty of the task by increasing the number of boxes. The study revealed key similarities between chimpanzee and human working memory. The best-performing chimpanzees remembered at least four items; one young chimpanzee remembered more than seven. They used both the appearance of the boxes as well as their position to remember their previous choices. – The Conversation.